Using Prediction Error to Account for the Pervasiveness of Mood Congruent Thoughts
Abstract
Depression represents one of the most common psychological difficulties across the globe, with 21.2% of people receiving a diagnosis of major depressive disorder at some point in their lives (Auerbach et al., 2018). One of the core symptoms of depression, and psychological difficulties more generally, concerns the possession of negatively valanced thoughts (Frewen et al., 2008; Hjemdal et al., 2013; Johnstone & Dallos, 2013; Myles et al., 2020; Westbrook et al., 2011). In a recent paper in the Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, Myles (2021) highlighted the critical importance of explicating the cognitive mechanisms underpinning psychological difficulties. This paper will discuss how the principles of associative learning can be used to account for the pervasiveness of negative thoughts in individuals with psychological difficulties.
Keywords
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Auerbach, R. P., Mortier, P., Bruffaerts, R., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Cuijpers, P., & Murray, E. (2018). WHO World Mental Health Surveys International College Student Project: Prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. Journal of abnormal psychology, 127(7), 623-638. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000362
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Stavraki, M., Lamprinakos, G., Wagner, B., & Díaz, D. (2018). Affective and cognitive validation of thoughts: An appraisal perspective on anger, disgust, surprise, and awe. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 693-718. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000118
Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(9), 393-399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.005
Clore, G. L., & Parrott, W. G. (2020). Moods and their vicissitudes: Thoughts and feelings as information. In Emotion and social judgments (pp. 107-123). Garland Science. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003058731-8
Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In Handbook of affect and social cognition, 121-144.
Dickinson, A. (2001). The 28th Bartlett memorial lecture causal learning: An associative analysis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 54(1b), 3-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724990042000010
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: the influence of emotion on trust. Journal of personality and social psychology, 88(5), 736-748. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736
Frewen, P. A., Evans, E. M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D. J., & Partridge, K. (2008). Letting go: Mindfulness and negative automatic thinking. Cognitive therapy and research, 32(6), 758-774. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9142-1
Frijda, N. H., Manstead, A. S., & Bem, S. E. (2000). Emotions and belief: How feelings influence thoughts. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511659904.001
Grossberg, S. (1982). Processing of expected and unexpected events during conditioning and attention: a psychophysiological theory. Psychological review, 89(5), 529-572. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.89.5.529
Haselgrove, M., & Hogarth, L. (Eds.). (2013). Clinical applications of learning theory. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203803509
Hjemdal, O., Stiles, T., & Wells, A. (2013). Automatic thoughts and meta‐cognition as predictors of depressive or anxious symptoms: A prospective study of two trajectories. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 54(2), 59-65. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12010
Johnstone, L., & Dallos, R. (2013). Formulation in psychology and psychotherapy: Making sense of people's problems. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203380574
Le Pelley, M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 57(3b), 193-243. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724990344000141
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological review, 82(4), 276-298. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076778
Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of personality and social psychology, 63(1), 119-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.119
Miller, R. R., Barnet, R. C., & Grahame, N. J. (1995). Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model. Psychological bulletin, 117(3), 363-386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.363
Myles, L. A. M. (2020). Robert Rescorla: A Legacy to Learning. Technology Networks. https://www.technologynetworks.com/neuroscience/articles/robert-rescorla-a-legacy-to-learning-336682
Myles, L. A. M. (2021). Opinion Article: The Emerging Role of Computational Psychopathology in Clinical Psychology. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.6092/2282-1619/mjcp-2895
Myles, L. A. M., Connolly, J., & Stanulewicz, N. (2020). The Mediating Role of Perceived Control and Desire for Control in the Relationship between Personality and Depression. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 8(3). https://doi.org/10.6092/2282-1619/mjcp-2589
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological review, 87(6), 532-552. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.87.6.532
Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2015). Emotion and persuasion: Cognitive and meta-cognitive processes impact attitudes. Cognition and Emotion, 29(1), 1-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.967183
Pham, M. T. (2004). The logic of feeling. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 360-369. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_5
Rescorla, R. A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Current research and theory, 64-99.
Roberts, I. D., & Hutcherson, C. A. (2019). Affect and decision making: insights and predictions from computational models. Trends in cognitive sciences, 23(7), 602-614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.005
Schultz, W., & Dickinson, A. (2000). Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual review of neuroscience, 23(1), 473-500. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473
Siegel, S., & Allan, L. G. (1996). The widespread influence of the Rescorla-Wagner model. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(3), 314-321. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210755
Tarrier, N., Wells, A., Haddock, G., & Davidson, J. (2000). Treating complex cases: The cognitive behavioural therapy approach. Chichester, England: John Wiley and Sons. https://doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.14.4.410
Taylor, E. W. (2001). Transformative learning theory: A neurobiological perspective of the role of emotions and unconscious ways of knowing. International Journal of lifelong education, 20(3), 218-236. https://doi.org/10.1080/02601370110036064
Walkenbach, J., & Haddad, N. F. (1980). The Rescorla-Wagner theory of conditioning: A review of the literature. The Psychological Record, 30(4), 497-509. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03394701
Westbrook, D., Kennerley, H., & Kirk, J. (2011). An introduction to cognitive behaviour therapy: Skills and applications. Sage. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1352465808004165
DOI: https://doi.org/10.13129/2282-1619/mjcp-3130
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.